Chicago School Readiness Project begins new wave of data collection

The Chicago School Readiness Project (CSRP) has begun a new wave of data collection in early 2019. Building on prior research, this effort looks to assess the long-term benefits of the project’s preschool intervention in Head Start, which launched in 2003. This initial intervention focused on supporting students’ self regulation and emotional regulation in preparation for upcoming years of schooling.

CSRP’s new round of research will focus on these same students, asking about their future plans as they enter adulthood. Most of the students are now 16-19 years old, nearing the end of high school, and deciding what their next endeavors will be. These students have accomplished a lot since the early years of the study — ending up in a variety of schools, pursuing individual activities and passions, with some participants even moving to different states. The researchers are now interested in hearing about which unique paths students will be taking in the coming years. Will students pursue further degrees? Are they enrolling in 4-year universities, community colleges, or vocational schools? Will they go into the workforce? What kind of work are they interested in?

The information gathered this year will provide valuable insights into the long-term effects of school interventions on student successes. CSRP students are more than just participants in a research study, they are collaborators. The researchers value their voices and want their stories to be heard. The information these participants provide will be used to inform other educational interventions and the future direction of research, which will help future generations of students to come. This information is not only interesting from an academic standpoint but is increasingly valuable in informing revision of policy and education standards.

Additionally, CSRP investigators Amanda Roy and Christine Li-Grining will soon be launching a new project with the same cohort. This investigation will provide a new intervention for the participants, helping them face the transition from teen to adult. In doing so, the researchers hope to provide support for the individuals’ burgeoning education and career goals.

March is National Nutrition Month — how does nutrition affect development?

Springtime is often associated with rebirth and renewal — the blooming plants and bright colors reminding us that as winter comes to a close, new life cycles begin. Thus, it seems appropriate that March is National Nutrition Month, a campaign from the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics to build awareness around issues of nutrition and health. The campaign’s goal is to educate and empower the American public to choose fresh, balanced options whenever possible. Thanks to former first-lady Michelle Obama’s childhood health campaign and the recent popularization of living a ‘healthy lifestyle’ – particularly through social media (1, 2) – issues of health and nutrition are more regularly talked about in recent years. Despite these changes, over 90% of the American public don’t manage to eat the recommended amounts of fresh fruits and vegetables, and obesity-related illnesses – like heart disease and diabetes – continue to be one of the larger causes of death across the country.

Poor nutrition is a huge issue for people across the globe, one that stems from a number of related, often systemic, issues, such as limited access to produce and fresh foods, undereducation about health and nutrition, and more broadly, poverty. Even though we often associate ‘malnutrition’ with populations in developing countries, a large number of children and adults across America suffer from food shortages every day. Researchers have shown that, along with a variety of other physical consequences, consistent deficiencies in nutrition have a significant impact on brain development and activity (3). There are two potential outcomes of poor nutrition – malnourishment and over-nourishment to the point of obesity – both of which have the potential to interfere with your brain’s well-being.

Malnutrition from not having enough food and/or enough nutritious food can have devastating impacts on neurological development. Research shows that deficiencies in micro and macronutrients during early childhood years (particularly protein-deficiencies) can lead to missed neurodevelopmental landmarks, which contribute to difficulty in learning, late school entry, and failure to perform at the same level as peers (4, 5). Other studies suggest that brains under starvation conditions will actively suppress certain functions to conserve energy. This process is called “down-regulation.” Our brains down-regulate certain functions as an energy-saving precaution so that necessary brain activities can continue for as long as possible, even when the body isn’t getting enough food. Studies of animal models suggest that memory is the primary function a brain will down-regulate during a period of starvation, in particular the formation of long-term memories (6). So, not only is malnutrition a risk-factor for developmental setbacks, but can also negatively impact our daily life and brain function!

On the other side of the issue, when overnutrition leads to obesity, there is also the potential for negative impacts on our brains. However, obesity-related risks tend to be more social and psychological, rather than physical. Researchers have shown that obesity in children and teens is associated with psychosocial stressors like stress, bullying, feelings of loneliness, isolation, or shame (7), and obese teens are more likely to engage in certain risky behaviors, like smoking cigarettes (8). Obesity is also associated with the development of mental health problems, like depression, in people of all ages (9). These findings are complicated, because psychosocial difficulties can also be influenced by a number of other factors, like genetic predisposition, living in poverty, living in unsafe neighborhoods, unstable home lives, high levels of stress, etc. (10). Further complicating the issue, some of those factors that influence the development of psychosocial difficulties can also be directly associated with obesity. The risks are all tangled together, making them hard to separate into individual causes and effects.

These are just a few of the potential risks that come from poor nutrition. Unfortunately, the reality is that many Americans just don’t have access to fresh food sources, either because of their location or socioeconomic status. Finding affordable produce can be challenging and expensive, and preparing that produce into home-cooked meals takes up a lot of time. Luckily, it’s not always as complicated as it may seem. Though we are bombarded daily with different opinions and facts about what foods may be ‘good’ and ‘bad’ for us, the reality that nutritionists hope to translate is that a healthy diet is a balanced diet. It doesn’t need to be fancy and it doesn’t have to be boring or bland. Every person’s daily intake of food should come from a mix of food groups including vegetables, fruits, grains, and protein sources, like chicken, fish or beans. There are so many combinations, the options are endless! The real key is to make sure you aren’t eating too much of certain kinds of foods – specifically meat, dairy products, and grains. And try to stay away from foods high in processed sugar and/or saturated fats. To find out more about choosing healthy options and reading food labels, check out this article.

Of course, regular exercise helps to keep your heart, mind, and body in good shape, as well! This can also be difficult to fit into a busy schedule, but even just taking a 15 minute walk every day, getting up to stretch and walk around every hour, or so, or parking at the far end of the supermarket parking lot to increase your daily steps can make a huge difference over time (11).

The National Nutrition Month website has lots of helpful information sheets with about how to build easy nutritious meals and snacks for yourself and your family. In addition, there are a ton of helpful tools available online to help guide your way to a healthier lifestyle! If you are looking for more tips and tricks, check out the government’s healthy-eating guidelines, the Department of Health & Human Services nutrition resources, and the Choose My Plate toolkit.
If you’re interested in learning more about demographics and food accessibility in your town, check out the Healthy Food Access Portal. You can also check out the Farm Aid page for resources to help navigate food labels and locate fresh produce near you!

The Family Life Project joins NIH-funded ECHO initiative

In 2015, the National Institutes of Health (NIH) pioneered a collaborative study hoping to pave the way to creating healthier environments for growing kids. This initiative, called the Environmental Influences on Child Health Outcomes (ECHO), aims to understand the effects of environmental factors — chemical and psychological — on children’s health and development. NIH researchers are particularly interested in assessing specific aspects of pediatric health, including upper and lower airways, obesity, pre-, peri-, and postnatal outcomes, and neurodevelopment, as well as the overall development of positive health outcomes. The collaboration brings together over 70 existing cohorts already enrolled in longitudinal studies across the country. In doing so, NIH hopes to build a standardized core of data — a data set large enough to tackle big questions about the early childhood environmental factors that determine major health outcomes.

Involvement in a collaborative study, like ECHO, allows for the associated research teams to maintain funding for their existing research projects while also contributing to the larger-scale data collection efforts of the overall project. For ECHO, collaboration allows NIH researchers to capitalize on existing longitudinal pediatric studies, grouping cohort data and standardizing data collection nationwide. This effort will bring together information from over 50,000 children and families from different backgrounds.  

The Family Life Project (FLP), a longitudinal study initiated in 2003 and lead by researchers from the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Pennsylvania State University, and NYU’s Neuroscience and Education Lab, joined the ECHO initiative in 2015. Doing so granted them funding to continue the project until at least 2023. FLP’s investigators hope their research will help to clarify how environmental factors influence child development in rural communities. In order to determine this, the researchers have been diligently following a cohort of families living in and around small towns in predominantly rural counties in North Carolina and Pennsylvania for the past 15 years. The FLP team meets with participating families regularly to develop an understanding of the environments in which they live, the challenges they face, and how their children are faring emotionally, physically, and academically.

Though FLP has now joined ECHO, the project’s research intentions remain the same. FLP investigators are primarily interested in the effects of early-life stress on the development of self-regulation in childhood. However, their data sets are large and far-reaching, and allow them to address questions about language development, school achievement, and various other aspects of physical and mental health throughout childhood and adolescence. Receiving ECHO funding will allow FLP to follow its cohort through adolescence and into early adulthood. As the participants grow older, FLP researchers will begin to explore the teens’ social lives, risk behaviors, and psychopathology.

The first two years of ECHO funding (2016-2018) gave the FLP team and their collaborators time to prepare for implementation of the ECHO-wide data collection protocol and to pool retrospective cohort data for ECHO’s analyses. 2019 will mark the first round of prospective data collection for ECHO cohorts, and the 16th data collection time point for FLP families. FLP’s involvement in ECHO means that these visits will look a bit different for FLP families than they have in previous years. This year, the families will be asked to attend a clinic visit, in lieu of having researchers visit them in their homes, where data collectors will be able to gather a larger variety of biospecimens including blood, urine, saliva, hair, stool and toenail clippings. Similar to previous data collection visits, the researchers will also conduct interviews and other tests of mental, physical, and social-emotional health. This data will continue to inform FLP investigators’ ongoing questions about child health and development, as well as contribute to ECHO’s nationwide data set.

Investigations such as ECHO and FLP undertake their research in a bid to improve child health for every family in the country, regardless of location and income-level. By developing a thorough understanding of how environmental and psycho-social factors can alter child development (both positively and negatively), longitudinal research provides the necessary information to instigate change. The findings of long-term studies of development, such as these, go on to promote new policies, regulations, and interventions that will best help children and young adults find happiness, health, and success.

Posted on |

New study investigates executive function predictors in infants aged 14 months

Can a baby’s attention span during its first few months serve as an indicator of his or her future abilities? A recent paper published in Child Development, co-authored by NEL’s Andrew Ribner, seeks to understand the relationship between behavioral abilities in early infancy and the emergence of executive function skills. Executive functions are cognitive abilities that help us regulate our emotions and behaviors and delay gratification — abilities which are essential for academic and personal success.

This research follows in the footsteps of similar studies, which have demonstrated that infant looking-time can be a significant indicator of later-life performance. Looking-time, a measure of infant visual attention, is a quality that varies significantly between individuals, and has been shown to predict intelligence into adulthood. Longer looking times signal slower processing speeds and as such, are related to intelligence. Looking time is often measured through procedures in which researchers hold up an attractive toy in front of an infant, measuring the time until the participant looks away from the object for at least 3 seconds. Traditionally, this procedure is repeated a number of times.

In this new study, led by Rory Devine of the University of Birmingham in the UK, the researchers hypothesized that looking time could also be used as a predictor of executive function ability later in life. They measured infant looking-times at 4 months old, and then returned 10 months later to assess the development of the infant’s executive function skills. At each visit, researchers used the established procedures for measuring executive function in infants. These measures include four tasks: one which assesses the infant’s ability to self-regulate by instructing them to not grab for attractive objects, two which measure their working memory by requiring infants to locate hidden toys in various scenarios and over multiple time-frames, and a fourth exercise that assesses the infant’s rule-following ability by asking them to repeat a set of tasks demonstrated by the examiner.

In regards to looking time, the researchers’ findings support those of former studies, demonstrating that infants with longer looking times performed worse in many measures of executive function than infants with shorter looking times. However, their results did not reveal consistent correlations between the measures of executive function and those of looking time. These results suggest that the measures may not effectively assess executive function in the desired age group due to inconsistency in demand between tasks, and/or variation in child development. A third possible conclusion from these results is that executive function skills, as we have come to understand them, are either established in later developmental stages, or that the components of executive function remain disparate skills until later in childhood.

Further studies are needed to gain a more complete understanding of executive function abilities in infants and toddlers. This new paper in Child Development, however, represents a major step forward in the measurement of these important abilities in very young children.

For more information about this study and its findings, check out the full paper.  

Posted on |

Collaboration with NEL and NYU’s Sullivan lab publish first cross-species paper

NEL’s postdoctoral research scientist, Rosemarie Perry, recently published her paper “Developing a neurobehavioral animal model of poverty: Drawing cross-species connections between environments of scarcity-adversity, parenting quality, and infant outcome.” Her work examines the mechanisms by which poverty affects development, and accomplishes this by comparing a rodent model of scarcity-adversity to results from a longitudinal study of human infants and families who faced high levels of scarcity adversity. The results from both studies indicate that parenting techniques can influence development, with positive and sensitive parenting ameliorating the negative effects that stress has on early child development.

Curious to learn more? Check out the full paper.

Posted on |

Expanding the Age of Adolescence: Implications for Criminal Justice Reform

In this article, PhD student Stephen Braren discusses expanding the period of time in which a person is considered an adolescent from ages 10-19 to 10-24, and the implications this could have on the juvenile justice system. He argues that advances in scientific research support this re-definition, with neuroimaging studies showing that key areas of the brain, involved in things like decision-making and emotion regulation, are still maturing in adolescence. In this article Stephen calls for support of the idea that science informs policy for the better.

Curious to learn more? Check out the full article.

Posted on |

FLP Releases Video

The Neuroscience and Education Lab’s Family Life Project, or FLP, is a federally-funded study which began in 2003 and is led by Dr. Clancy Blair. FLP follows 1,292 children living in rural counties in Pennsylvania and North Carolina and strives to uncover the developmental mechanisms behind child self-regulation and stress response physiology in family, peer, school, and neighborhood contexts. The FLP team brings together researchers with expertise in education, medicine, psychology, sociology, anthropology, geography and human development. Now that the FLP children are becoming teenagers, they are becoming more curious about the purpose and findings of FLP. In a recent survey we gave them, many FLP teens expressed wanting to learn more about the study. One teen stated, “I’d love to see and keep track of the project and it’s research done so far. I find it very interesting and would love to see what I have contributed to.” The FLP team makes it a priority that all of the teens’ voices are heard and their questions answered. To reflect this, we created a short video to explain the meaning behind their participation in FLP.

To learn more check out FLP’s video!

Posted on |